A Study on Meditation Psycho-Physiological Consequences

Exploring the Impact of Meditation on Mind and Body

by Dr. Ajay Dubey*,

- Published in International Journal of Physical Education & Sports Sciences, E-ISSN: 2231-3745

Volume 13, Issue No. 5, Aug 2018, Pages 36 - 41 (6)

Published by: Ignited Minds Journals


ABSTRACT

Substantial clinical literature on meditation as alternative mind-body treatment has taken place during the past four decades. This paper aims to provide an analysis of the existing state of meditation and wellness studies. It analysed recent meditation studies and their impact on multiple illnesses. There are two primary forms of meditation (concentration and attentiveness) actually dominating. Effects such as pulse, blood pressure, corrective action, digestion, ventilation, and resilience of the skin are addressed in the meditation. It also discusses the impact of meditation on human vision and cognition. There is also talk about potential means of or processes by which meditation may have health benefits such as calming, systemic desensitization, release of repressed memories, tension free, etc. Finally, significant philosophical and analytical questions are dealt with which researchers in this sector ought to pay serious attention to potential studies.

KEYWORD

meditation, psycho-physiological consequences, mind-body treatment, wellness studies, concentration, attentiveness, pulse, blood pressure, corrective action, digestion, ventilation, resilience of the skin, human vision, cognition, calming, systemic desensitization, release of repressed memories, tension free, philosophical questions, analytical questions

INTRODUCTION

A variety of experiments have been carried out with some accuracy of the findings obtained that investigate the psychological concurrent meditation. The influence of the anticipation and incentive of success in the participants of meditators is challenging to monitor by utilising subjective reporting of psychological activity. However, several scientific reviews – both psychological and medical – indicate important results and include the interesting associations with the above summarised observations for meditation and brain function in collaboration with the other psychological research. The main field of mediation and meditation is the psychology, but very little empirical evaluations were made of meditation and concentration. Longitudinal studies in children and adults for breath-focused meditation have recorded improved performance on the integrated figures test which enable the individual to ignore distracting stimuli. A cross-sectional analysis of TM children and a cohort of age and gender controls showed that meditation contributed to better treatment interventions. Attention and concentration activities were contrasted by means of an auditory counting exercise that is likely to slow attention. In contrast to monitors, superior focus efficiency has been attained in comparison to short-term meditator status. In a second task which assessed ongoing attention to unwanted stimuli, conscience meditationists also showed better output than concentric mediators. In comparison, the short-term mediation results on a focusing activity indicated that TM did not result in an increase in attention functioning, which correlates to the explicit lack of concentrate on concentrating effort utilising the technology. The above-mentioned CNV studies reinforce the opinion that long-term TM mediators are more likely to take charge of controls. Since meditation is a type of care preparation, neurophysiological conclusions include enhanced activation in the frontal care system, and more research are required to validate this hypothesis. A similar clinical research investigated the effect of concentrative yogic yoga on the care neglect hyperactivity disorder in teenagers, with reports showing major changes in symptoms during a six-week training procedure. The 'absorption' psychological characteristic applies to treatment and is important to meditative activity. Absorption applies to episodes of complete focus requiring processes of representational capital contributing to temporary situations of distorted self-perception and reality view. The results show that absorption and avoidance of distress are independent of meditative abilities, although it is not obvious if this is related to or due to a predisposition to meditation practise (Davidson, Goleman, & Schwartz, 1976). Further studies evaluating mediators' neurophysiological activity in terms of absorption can help characterised person differences for the spectrum of brain and mind responses to meditative training.

broad understanding that meditation could contribute to changes in perceptual acuity and/or care, however detailed perception tests are uncommon. An analysis of perceptively unclear visual stimuli with a binocular challenge has shown that one-point concentric meditation will stabilize one of perceptual awareness opportunities. More German than the observations of enriching perceptive clarity, the visual exposure threshold was lower for mediators than controls and a three-month intensive retreat for the meditation appeared to yield further threshold decreases. Studies in yogic concentrative meditation also shown that, in contrast to test groups that have not received such instruction, there are changes in vital flicker-fusions following training for adolescents, young adults and adults. Visual contrast vulnerability also increased in a population of epileptic adults secondary to Sahaj Yoga training. A further perceptive acuity research, ideally coupled with neurophysiologic testing, involves long-standing explanations of the increase of the percipient area through meditation combined with the indicative effects studied here and the continuity with event-related likely outcomes. The belief that meditative exercise will reduce the impact of anxiety and stress on psychic and physiological activity is confirmed by a large number of researchers. The central nervous system's adaptive plasticity makes large neurophysiological disorders shifts, which may develop into trait manifestations after long hours of work, stylized focus, perceptual contexts reframing, and the relational rules involved in meditative exercise. This is consistent with the linkages between increased tension, increased corticosteroid levels and hippocampal neurogenesis inhibition. Meditation is linked to decreased amounts of cortisol and catecholamine. Some meditator studies have measured physiological reactions to traumatic triggers, which are particularly important considering the supposed advantages of lower automation and reactivity coupled with improved calm and meditation compassion. After exposure to traumatic video clips, meditators demonstrated a swift return to normal for cardiac and skin behaviour. Meditators were also found to lack frontal gamma induction in reaction to traumatic film clips for non-meditators. This experiments are tentative, yet encourage neurophysiological responses to socially difficult triggers to proceed. Patient practices have provided promising clinical outcomes for fear, immune-protection research, pain and stress-related skin disorders. These findings are compatible with the hypothesizing that meditation contributes to a major reorganization of frontal hemisphere behaviour, linked maybe to the rise in theta and alpha EEG stimulation. emotional reactivity. It may be beneficial under these situations to acquire neurophysiological tests in order to characterised neuronal mediating factors correlated with therapeutic enhancement by evaluating the medical and/or psychological outcome. For eg, reported left-over-right asymmetry changes in frontal behaviour correlated with an increase in immune measurements after secondary meditation training and an increase in auditory amplitude in P300 correlated with change in depression after yogic meditation. More studies into mediation and the biochemical processes of stress/emotional reactivity will help the hypotheses of stress-relating limit structure reorganization in a practical way. Meditative activities that utilize internal role play to produce some enduring emotions or intentions of love/compassion have started to be discussed. Neuroimaging approaches have not fully investigated the impact of meditation on the relational function, but psychiatric trials indicate that psychological focus varying with meditative activity is improved and that exposure to distress tends to be mitigated strongly. Cognitive therapy, which typically involves thoughtful reflection, was especially effective in managing depression. In fact. The particular results occur in patients who have have three or more past depressive episodes are linked to depression reduction. The most correlated psychological variable with enhanced resilience to depression is the 'musicological consciousness,' the change towards suicidal feelings as measurable brain contents rather than the self. As with tension, depression is related to enhanced cortisol and reduced neurogenesis in hippocampes. The rise in the metacognitive consciousness that seems to be consistent with the success of therapeutic interventions is challenging to reconcile with existing neuroimaging evidence, however it does seem to contribute to the basic objectives of meditation for creating sustainable results for the interaction between self and non-self. A collection of experimental paradigms that attempt to test the subtleties of self referenzal care in health and disease have recently been created, so that more psychometrical statements on improvements in self-experience may be quantified with brain dependent measures. To recognise the neurophysiological and psychological changes triggered by meditative practise, it is important to determine the psychometric characteristics and states. For both state and characteristic shifts, multiple investigators have developed certain steps. This form of characteristic study shows, after a meditative preparation and correlated with the perception of well-being, that the psychological heterogeneity altered statements of mind into a four-dimensional state space composed of triggering, periods of consciousness, self-awareness and sensory complex constructions. This method includes signatures from experienced state that can map neurophysiological variations more easily than higher dimensional spaces, but this narrow space in four dimensions does not sufficiently answer the whole spectrum of changes caused by meditation. Given the broad variety of potential meditations and the subsequent states, multiple activities appear likely to contribute to various therapeutic results and even different kinds of psychological shifts. In reality, recent studies have shown that novices in zen meditation display low-trait anxiety associated with frontal alpha accuracy effects, while new study findings linked to frontal alpha strength increase and frontal theta correlated harm mitigation rates increase. Ses observations are preliminary in nature, but they act as a theoretically useful paradigm for how meditation neurophysiology may be correlated to psychological conditions. Quantifying characteristic changes produced by numerous mental sets can promote insight into the psychological impacts of meditation by rigorously comparing the techniques required to define particular psychological effects.

Further course for the future

As mentioned above, numerous recent findings have shown that different meditation methods contribute to various neurophypophysiological effects, in order to isolate the functioning brain behaviour of psychological conditions by contrasting the meditative activities with the other methods of altered state induction. To improve the therapeutic efficacy of these approaches it would be important to determine personality improvements, clinical effects and condition trait-neuroactivity markers in meditative practise. Targeted theta, alpha, and gamma strength tests as well as cohesion results can assist the development of a data base for potential uses both in state and function research of meditation. The strong quantitative distinction and topographical mapping of the gap in meditation and early sleep periods remains a major challenge for simple meditation studies. Early drowning or sleeping symptoms are the most prominent state consequences of meditation periods of alpha and theta enhancement. Increases in theta power in certain long-term meditators can be attributed to learning to be receptive to sleep stage I on an equivalent, though not similar, degree of physiological therapy. Maintaining information may raise consciousness even as deep sleep progresses impacting the neurophysiological markers correlated with sleep. This theory offers a phenomenological link between slow behaviour in meditation practices and regular sleep which indicate the distribution of theta versus alpha changes in power, increase in theta and alpha consistency in meditation vs. decreases in sleep, and likely the increase in low-frequency power associated with meditation which is reduced in slope The theta increase is the frontal midline theta provided by the previous cingulate, dorsal, and medial prefrontal cortices. The topic usually seen during the transition from stage I to stage II sleep is less consistent over time and comes from more traditional sources. A detailed empirical differentiation between these two improved theta states may include a much required distinction between meditative and sleep phenomenologies.

CONCLUSION

Neuroelectrical and other neuroimaging techniques are used to investigate meditation conditions and features. Though an integral methodological and theoretical basis is still evolving, the outcomes would be more consistent and guided. Meditation explicitly influences the role of the central nervous system, but neurological variations and inconsistencies within activities are far from simple. For prospective research, the possibility that meditation exercise would be clinically helpful in combination with psychiatric and neuropharmacological therapies. The current analysis seeks to prepare the way for this advancement with an organized, up-to-date overview of how meditation influences the brain.

REFERENCES

Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2005). Impact of regular meditation practice on EEG activity at rest and during evoked negative emotions. International Journal of Neuroscience, 115, pp. 893–909. Beauchamp-Turner, D. L., & Levinson, D. M. (1992). Effects of meditation on stress, health, and affect. Medical Psychotherapy: An International Journal, 5, pp. 123–131. Carlson, L. E., Speca, M., Patel, K. D., & Goodey, E. (2003). mindfulness based stress reduction in relation to quality of life, mood, symptoms of stress, and immune parameters in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, pp. 571–581 Carlson, L. E., Speca, M., Patel, K. D., & Goodey, E. (2004). Mindfulness based stress reduction in relation to quality of life, mood, symptoms of stress and levels of cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) and melatonin in breast and prostate cancer

Davidson, R. J. (2003). Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: Toward a synthesis. Psychophysiology, 40, pp. 655– 665. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, pp. 33– 47 Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, pp. 144 –158. Kabat-Zinn, J., Lipworth, L., & Burney, R. (1985). The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 8, pp. 163–190. Kabat-Zinn, J., Wheeler, E., Light, T., Skillings, A., Scharf, M. J., Cropley, T. G., et al. (1998). Influence of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention on rates of skin clearing in patients with moderate to severe psoriasis undergoing phototherapy (UVB) and photochemotherapy (PUVA). Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, pp. 625– 632. Miller, J. J., Fletcher, K., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1995). Three-year follow-up and clinical implications of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention in the treatment of anxiety disorders. General Hospital Psychiatry, 17, pp. 192–200. Shapiro, S. L., & Walsh, R. (2003). An analysis of recent meditation research and suggestions for future directions. Humanistic Psychologist, 31, pp. 86 –114. Dunn, B. R., Hartigan, J. A., & Mikulas, W. L. (1999). Concentration and mindfulness meditations: Unique forms of consciousness? Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 24, pp. 147–165. Lazar, S. W., Rosman, I. S., Vangel, M., Rao, V., Dusek, H., Benson, H., et al. (2003, November). Functional brain imaging of mindfulness and mantra-based meditation. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans, LA. Castillo-Richmond, A., Schneider, R. H., Alexander, C. N., Cook, R., Myers, H., Nidich, S., et al. (2000). Effects of stress reduction on carotid atherosclerosis in hypertensive African Americans. Stroke, 31, pp. 568 –573. Sudarshan Kriya yoga (SKY). Journal of Affective Disorders, 50, pp. 45–48. Schneider, R. H., Staggers, F., Alexander, C. N., Sheppard, W., Rainforth, M., Kondwani, K., et al. (1995). A randomized controlled trial of stress reduction for hypertension in older African Americans. Hypertension, 26, 820 – 827. Zamarra, J. W., Schneider, R. H., Besseghini, I., Robinson, D. K., & Salerno, J. W. (1996). Usefulness of the transcendental meditation program in the treatment of patients with coronary artery disease. American Journal of Cardiology, 77, pp. 867– 870. Gelderloos, P., Walton, K. G., Orme-Johnson, D. W., & Alexander, C. N. (1991). Effectiveness of the transcendental meditation program in preventing and treating substance misuse: A review. International Journal of the Addictions, 26, pp. 293–325. Jevning, R., Wallace, R. K., & Beidebach, M. (1992). The physiology of meditation: A review. A wakeful hypometabolic integrated response. Neuroscience and Bio behavioral Reviews, 16, pp. 415–424 Walton, K. G., Pugh, N. D., Gelderloos, P., & Macrae, P. (1995). Stress reduction and preventing hypertension: Preliminary support for a psychoneuroendocrine mechanism. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 1, pp. 263–283. Murthy, P. J., Gangadhar, B. N., Janakiramaiah, N., & Subbakrishna, D. K. (1997). Normalization of P300 amplitude following treatment in dysthymia. Biological Psychiatry, 42, pp. 740–743. Esch, T., Guarna, M., Bianchi, E., Zhu, W., & Stefano, G. B. (2004). Commonalities in the central nervous system‟s involvement with complementary medical therapies: Limbic morphinergic processes. Medical Science Monitor, 10, pp. MS6 –17. Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L. R., & Pizzagalli, D. (2001). Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Research, 108, pp. 111–121. during three meditative states: Preliminary data. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans, LA. Ma, S. H., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: Replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, pp. 31– 40. Mason, O., & Hargreaves, I. (2001). A qualitative study of mindfulness based cognitive therapy for depression. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 74, pp. 197–212. Rohan, K. J. (2003). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse and overcoming resistance in cognitive therapy. Psychiatry: Interpersonal & Biological Processes, 66, pp. 272–281 Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: Guilford Press Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training help? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, pp. 25–39. Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, pp. 615–623. Brown, E. S., Rush, A. J., & McEwen, B. S. (1999). Hippocampal remodeling and damage by corticosteroids: Implications for mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, pp. 474 – 484. Gould, E., Tanapat, P., Rydel, T., & Hastings, N. (2000). Regulation of hippocampal neurogenesis in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 48, pp. 715–720. Jacobs, B. L. (2002). Adult brain neurogenesis and depression. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 16, pp. 602–609. Malberg, J. E., & Duman, R. S. (2003). Cell proliferation in adult hippocampus is decreased by inescapable stress: Reversal Thomas, R. M., & Peterson, D. A. (2003). A neurogenic theory of depression gains momentum. Molecular Intervention, 3, pp. 441– 444. Vollmayr, B., Simonis, C., Weber, S., Gass, P., & Henn, F. (2003). Reduced cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus is not correlated with the development of learned helplessness. Biological Psychiatry, 54, pp. 1035– 1040. Austin, J. H. (2000). Consciousness evolves when the self dissolves. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, pp. 209 –230. Levenson, M. R., Jennings, P. A., Aldwin, C. M., & Shiraishi, R. W. (2005). Self-transcendence: Conceptualization and measurement. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 60, pp. 127–143. Walsh, R. (1982). The original goals of meditation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, pp. 1525–1526. Kircher, T. T., & David, A. (Eds.). (2003). The self in neuroscience and psychiatry. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Kircher, T. T., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Benson, P. J., Bullmore, E. T., & Brammer, M., et al. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self-processing: Effects of faces and words. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, pp. 133–144. Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjaer, T. W., et al. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, pp. 6827– 6832. Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G., & Mohamed, F. B. (2004). Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, pp. 114 –122. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, pp. 822– 848. Buchheld, N., Grossman, P., & Walach, H. (2001). Measuring mindfulness in insight meditation (Vipassana) and meditation-based psychotherapy: The development of the

Ott, U. (2001). The EEG and the depth of meditation. Journal for Meditation and Meditation Research, 1, pp. 55– 68 Piron, H. (2001). The meditation depth index (MEDI) and the meditation depth questionnaire (MEDEQ). Journal for Meditation and Meditation Research, 1, pp. 69 –92. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, pp. 822– 848 Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J., Jamieson, G. A., Kotchoubey, B., Kubler, A., et al. (2005). Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 131, pp. 98 –127. Travis, F., Arenander, A., & DuBois, D. (2004). Psychological and physiological characteristics of a proposed object-referral/self-referral continuum of self-awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, pp. 401– 420 Wilber, K., Engler, J., & Brown, D. P. (1986). Transformations of consciousness: Conventional and contemplative perspectives on development. Boston: Shambhala Murata, T., Takahashi, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Yoshida, H., & Wada, Y. (2004). Individual trait anxiety levels characterizing Takahashi, T., Murata, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Kikuchi, M., et al.. (2005). Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 55, pp. 199 –207. Corby, J. C., Roth, W. T., Zarcone, V. P., Jr., & Kopell, B. S. (1978). Psychophysiological correlates of the practice of tantric yoga meditation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, pp. 571–577. Pagano, R. R., Rose, R. M., Stivers, R. M., & Warrenburg, S. (1976). Sleep during transcendental meditation. Science, 191, pp. 308 –310. Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques, and Younger, J., Adriance, W., & Berger, R. J. (1975). Sleep during transcendental meditation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 40, pp. 953–954. Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2001). Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: High-resolution EEG investigation of meditation. Neuroscience Letters, 310, pp. 57– 60. Asada, H., Fukuda, Y., Tsunoda, S., Yamaguchi, M., & Tonoike, M. (1999). Frontal midline theta rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 274, pp. 29 –32. Hebert, R., & Lehmann, D. (1977). Theta bursts: An EEG pattern in normal subjects practicing the transcendental meditation technique. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42, pp. 397– 405. Ishii, R., Shinosaki, K., Ukai, S., Inouye, T., Ishihara, T., Yoshimine, T., et al. (1999). Medial prefrontal cortex generates frontal midline theta rhythm. NeuroReport, 10, pp. 675– 679.

Corresponding Author Dr. Ajay Dubey*

Assistant Professor, Govt. Girls‟ P.G College, Etawah